Playing before words: sensory explorations and the emergence of play in the perinatal period

Journal title INTERAZIONI
Author/s Sara Micotti
Publishing Year 2023 Issue 2023/1 Language Italian
Pages 17 P. 124-140 File size 213 KB
DOI 10.3280/INT2023-001010
DOI is like a bar code for intellectual property: to have more infomation click here

Below, you can see the article first page

If you want to buy this article in PDF format, you can do it, following the instructions to buy download credits

Article preview

FrancoAngeli is member of Publishers International Linking Association, Inc (PILA), a not-for-profit association which run the CrossRef service enabling links to and from online scholarly content.

As psychoanalysts of the family group with babies, we have the opportunity to work on the relationship between the parents and the child, when everything is formed: the child’s self, the parental identity, the relationship. The work of observation, imagination, play, storytelling can transform anxieties into tolerable emotions and make resources and healthy virtualities ac-cessible. Where there was confusion, emptiness and fog, reference points, metaphors, narra-tives can take shape in the sessions. In working with children in the first year of life ‒ when communication takes place predominantly through the body, sensory skills, motor skills ‒ we can intervene at the origins, in moments of disconnection, when the trauma prevents parents from reading the children’s communicative signals and leads children to loss of interest and relationship avoidance. Therapy can lead children to express strong communicative cues and learn about emotions, and it can interest parents in contacting themselves and discovering the children’s internal world. It can offer moments of reparation, unison, thought, which increase hope and a sense of security.

Keywords: archaic anxieties, preverbal narration, translation, playing, parent-infant psycho-therapy.

  1. Alvarez A. (1992). Live Company: psychoanalytic psychotherapy with autistic, borderline, deprived and abused children. London: Routledge.
  2. Ammaniti M., Ferrari P.F. (2020). Il corpo non dimentica. L’Io motorio e lo sviluppo della relazionalità. Milano: Raffaello Cortina.
  3. Avdi E., Baradon T. et al. (2020). Studying the process of psychoanalytic parent–infant psychotherapy: Embodied and discursive aspects. Infant Mental Health Journal, 41, 5: 589-602.
  4. Bion W.R. (1962). Apprendere dall’esperienza. Roma: Armando, 1972.
  5. Bion W.R. (1965). Trasformazioni. Roma: Armando, 1973.
  6. Bion W.R. (1970). Attenzione e interpretazione. Roma: Armando, 1973.
  7. Bolognini S. (2001). “É solo accaduto…”: un breve commento a La Capacità di sorprendersi di Lore Schacht. Richard e Piggle, 2, 9: 131-136.
  8. Civitarese G. (2013). Spettri del transfert. In Ferro A. (a cura di), Psicoanalisi oggi. Roma: Carocci.
  9. Civitarese G., Ferro A. (2013). The meaning and use of metaphor in analytic field theory. Psychoanalytic Inquiry. 33: 190-209. DOI: 10.1080/07351690.2013.77988
  10. Cramer B., Palacio-Espasa F. (1993). La pratique des psychothèrapies mères-bèbès. Études cliniques et techniques. Paris: PUF.
  11. Dolto F. (1994). Tout est langage. Paris: Gallimard.
  12. Ferro A. (2013). Modello onirico della mente. In Ferro A. (a cura di), Psicoanalisi oggi. Roma: Carocci.
  13. Fraiberg S., Adelson E., Shapiro V. (1975). I fantasmi nella stanza dei bambini. In Fraiberg S., Il sostegno allo sviluppo. Milano: Raffaello Cortina, 1999.
  14. Freud A. (1927). Four lectures on child analysis. In Freud A., Writings of Anna Freud 1. New York: International Universities Press. 
  15. Freud S. (1920). Al di là del principio di piacere. OSF, vol. 9. Torino: Bollati Boringhieri.
  16. Golse B. (2006). L’essere bebè. Milano: Raffaello Cortina, 2008.
  17. Kaës R. (2009). Les alliances inconscientes. Paris: Dunod.
  18. Klein M. (1926). The psychological principles of early analysis. In Klein M., Love, guilt and reparation and other works, 1921-1945. London: The Hogarth Press, 1975.
  19. Maiello S. (2011). Dialoghi ante litteram. Note sugli elementi ritmici e sonori del linguaggio e della comunicazione verbale. Richard e Piggle, 19, 3: 246-266.
  20. Maiello S. (2012). Lo sviluppo del gioco e del linguaggio. In Maiello S. (Ed.), Gioco e linguaggio. Roma: Astrolabio
  21. Massaglia P. (2022). Comunicazione personale.
  22. Nicolò A.M. (2014). Where is the unconscious located? Reflections on links in families and couples. In Nicolò A.M., Benghozi P., Lucarelli D. (Eds.), Families in transformation. A psychoanalytic approach. London: Karnak.
  23. Nicolò A.M. (2015). Dov’è collocato l’inconscio? Riflessioni sui legami nelle coppie e nelle famiglie. Interazioni, 41, 1: 33-42. DOI: 10.3280/INT2015-00100
  24. Norman J. (2004). Le trasformazioni di esperienze infantili precoci: un bambino di sei mesi in psicoanalisi. L’annata Psicoanalitica Internazionale, 2/2006: 131-151.
  25. Ogden T.H. (2016). Destruction reconceived: On Winnicott’s “The Use of an Object and Relating through Identifications”. The International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 97, 5: 1243-1262. DOI: 10.1111/1745-8315.1255
  26. Provenzi L. et al. (2023). Prenatal maternal stress during the COVID-19 pandemic and infant regulatory capacity at 3 months: A longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 35, 1: 35-43. DOI: 10.1017/S095457942100076
  27. Salomonsson B. (2011). La musica del contenimento. Come e quando rivolgersi ai membri della diade madre-lattante in trattamento psicoanalitico. Interazioni, 34, 2: 29-47. DOI: 10.3280/INT2011-00200
  28. Salomonsson B. (2014). Psychoanalytic therapy with infants and parents. Practice, theory and results. London: Routledge.
  29. Salomonsson B. (2017). The function of language in parent-infant psychotherapy. The International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 98, 6: 1597-1618. DOI: 10.1111/1745-8315.1266
  30. Schacht L. (2001). La capacità di sorprendersi. Richard e Piggle, 2, 9: 117-130.
  31. Shai D., Belsky J. (2017). Parental embodied mentalizing: how the nonverbal dance between parents and infants predicts children’s socio-emotional functioning. Journal of Attachment and Human Development, 19, 2: 191-219. DOI: 10.1080/14616734.2016.125565
  32. Silverman R.C., Lieberman A.F. (1999). Negative maternal attributions, projective identification, and the intergenerational transmission of violent relational patterns. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 9, 2: 161-186. DOI: 10.1080/1048188990953931
  33. Spagnolo R. (2022). Comunicazione personale.
  34. Spagnolo R., Northhoff G. (2022). Il sé dinamico in psicoanalisi. Fondamenti neuroscientifici e clinica psicoanalitica. Milano: FrancoAngeli.
  35. Stern D. (1985). Il mondo interpersonale del bambino. Torino: Bollati Boringhieri, 1987.
  36. Stern D. (1990). Diario di un bambino. Milano: Mondadori, 1991.
  37. Terr L. (1988). What happens to early memories of trauma? A study of twenty children under age five at the time of documented traumatic events. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 1: 96-104. DOI: 10.1097/00004583-198801000-0001
  38. Trevarthen C. (2005). First things first: Infants make good use of the sympathetic rhythm of imitation, without reason or language. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 31, 1: 91-113. DOI: 10.1080/0075417050007965
  39. Tronick E. (2008). Regolazione emotiva. Milano: Raffaello Cortina.
  40. Tustin F. (1986). Autistic barriers in neurotic patients. London: Routledge.
  41. Winnicott D.W. (1947). L’odio nel controtransfert. In Winnicott D.W., Dalla pediatria alla psicoanalisi. Firenze: Martinelli, 1975.
  42. Winnicott D.W. (1971). Oggetti transizionali e fenomeni transizionali. In Winnicott D.W., Gioco e realtà. Roma: Armando, 1974.

Sara Micotti, Giocare prima delle parole: esplorazioni sensoriali ed emergere del gioco nel periodo perinatale in "INTERAZIONI" 1/2023, pp 124-140, DOI: 10.3280/INT2023-001010